WHERE HUMANS AND NATURE COLLIDE

Friday 24 February 2012

Ambient noise may impact parent-offspring communication


To understand why anthropogenic noise has the potential to negatively impact animals--a topic visited often here on Anthrophysis--it’s helpful to know a bit about signal detection theory (SDT). First applied to animal behavior by bioacoustician extraordinaire Haven Wiley, SDT recognizes that it can be biologically costly either to miss an important signal or to respond to one that you shouldn’t. The presence of anthropogenic noise makes the former scenario more likely because it threatens to mask relevant sounds. Animals may react to this by “lowering their response threshold,” or responding more readily to any sound that seems remotely important—thus leading to the latter scenario. Both can have serious negative implications.

Such errors are particularly likely in juveniles learning the ropes of acoustic communication. Nestling birds, which use acoustic signals to determine when they should beg for food from their parents, may fail to respond to the adults' audible arrivals as often as 20% of the time, and may accidentally beg to a non-parental noise as much as 50% of the time. The first mistake could cost them a meal, and, if passing predators use the vocalizations to locate and prey on a nest, the second could cost their lives.

 
(Tree swallow, Tachycineta bicolor, peeking out of its nest box)


The likelihoods of these different scenarios were recently investigated by two biologists from Nova Scotia’s Dalhousie University. Using tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) as their focal species, the researchers quantified whether the presence of environmental noise increases the frequency of begging errors. To do this, they found 28 swallow nests with 4 chicks apiece; once the chicks reached 10-12 days of age, two individuals per nest were allocated to a noise treatment, while the other two were used as controls. For each trial, pairs of birds were removed from their home nest box and placed in an unoccupied "test" box. For one hour, chicks in the test box were either exposed to 65 dB of white noise (noise treatment) or left alone (control). During the next 20 minutes, while the original acoustic conditions continued, the birds were also exposed to 7 presentations of 3 audio recordings: a parent landing on a nest box and uttering a contact call to stimulate begging; a parent landing on a box but making no additional sounds; and a potential predator (common grackle, Quiscalis versicolor) landing on the nest box. Each trial was filmed, and the researchers later watched the resulting videos in order to observe the amount of begging the chicks performed in response to the different stimuli and ambient noise conditions.

As predicted, nestlings begged more often to the parent-with-call stimulus (96% of trials) than to the parent-without-call stimulus (54% of trials). Responses to both stimuli were significantly lower among birds in the noise treatment, indicating that the parental acoustic signals were being masked.  However, chick response levels to predator noises were similar (35%) in both the noise and control treatments. This suggests that noise-exposed birds had not lowered their response thresholds in response to the ambient noise conditions.

(Common grackle, Quiscalis versicolor)


Chicks that delay, or fail to perform, a begging display in response to a parent’s arrival risk being outcompeted by a more vociferous sibling; thus, it is important to respond both quickly and loudly once an adult lands at the nest box. Although there may be significant costs associated with failing to beg appropriately, it may be even worse to “jump the gun” and beg in response to noises that turn out to have been made by a non-parent. This may be why the tree swallows in this study failed to lower their response threshold despite the presence of noise pollution. However, the authors point out the possibility that there may be other mechanisms by which the negative impacts of noise are mitigated—for instance, the presence of “visual or tactile cues…[that] signal parental arrivals,” or the use of post-arrival parental vocalizations to stimulate begging.

In a previous study on nestling tree swallows, these same two researchers found that noise-exposed chicks did not suffer any decreases in feeding or growth rates. Thus, even if reductions in signal detection lead to missed feedings or redistribution of parental care among different siblings, this does not appear to have any long-term consequences chicks of this species. However, the scientists suggest that nestlings may owe their good health to their parents—who might compensate for noise-induced feeding problems by expending additional energy on chick care, spending less time looking after their own needs, or both; similar patterns have been reported for birds exposed to other types of anthropogenic disturbance.

(Tree swallow nestlings in their feather-lined nest box)


The many remaining questions associated with this topic reveal just how much there is to learn about anthropogenic noise and its effects on wildlife. For instance, it will be vital to evaluate whether younger chicks (who rely more on acoustic cues until their eyes open at ~day 5) are more susceptible to ambient noise, to quantify whether noise disturbance is particularly costly to adults attempting to rear broods, and to perform studies on additional breeding bird species.

---
Leonard, M.L. and Horn, A.G. 2012. Ambient noise increases missed detections in nestling birds. Biology Letters, online advance publication.

Thanks to the following websites for providing the images used in this post:
http://www.carolinabirdclub.org/gallery/Lewis/tres_2.html 
http://billhubick.com/photos/birds/common_grackle.php
http://meadowblog.typepad.com/mblog/2008/07/tree-swallows-a.html

No comments:

Post a Comment